1422 Segmental & Suprasegmental Errors of Nonword ... - ASHA [PDF]

Segmental and supra-segmental accuracy of nonword repetition in bilingual children ... Investigated NWR between bilingua

10 downloads 14 Views 813KB Size

Recommend Stories


Suprasegmental Errors, Pronunciation Instruction and Communication
If your life's work can be accomplished in your lifetime, you're not thinking big enough. Wes Jacks

Suprasegmental units of phonetics
You miss 100% of the shots you don’t take. Wayne Gretzky

ASHA
Don't be satisfied with stories, how things have gone with others. Unfold your own myth. Rumi

ehi325_online-only 1422..1448
Life is not meant to be easy, my child; but take courage: it can be delightful. George Bernard Shaw

G0255 (1422 K)
Learning never exhausts the mind. Leonardo da Vinci

ASHA 2016
And you? When will you begin that long journey into yourself? Rumi

ehi325_online-only 1422..1448
Keep your face always toward the sunshine - and shadows will fall behind you. Walt Whitman

Analysis of Errors Analysis of Errors
Be who you needed when you were younger. Anonymous

The Spoken English of Hong Kong: A Study of Co-occurring Segmental Errors
Don't be satisfied with stories, how things have gone with others. Unfold your own myth. Rumi

Pfaff 422 1422 720 04
Never wish them pain. That's not who you are. If they caused you pain, they must have pain inside. Wish

Idea Transcript


Segmental and supra-segmental accuracy of nonword repetition in bilingual children ASHA Convention Atlanta, GA 2012

Sue Ann Lee, Sherry Sancibrian, & Dalan Gore

Disclosure • No financial or non-financial interest to disclose.

Background • Nonword repetition (NWR) has been widely used for evaluating phonological short-term memory. • In clinical practice, NWR tasks have been recommended as a component of less-biased child language assessment protocols.

Previous studies • Gathercole & Baddeley (1990) ▫ Nonword repetition in 6 children with SLI and 6 age-matched and younger children with typical development. ▫ The children with SLI scored significantly lower than both groups of children with typical development.

Previous studies • Thal, Miller, Carlson, & Vega (2005) ▫ 44 typically developing and 20 children that had a history of language delay were studied. ▫ Children that had a history of language delay scored significantly lower than children with typical language development.

Previous studies • Munson, Edwards, & Beckman (2005) ▫ Compared NWR of typically developing children to children with phonological disorders ▫ Children with phonological disorders were less accurate overall, but both groups repeated high frequency sequences of phonemes more successfully than low frequency.

Previous studies • Calderon & Guiterrez-Clellen (2003) ▫ Compared NWR performance of 21 Spanishspeaking children with SLI with that of 21 typically developing Spanish-speaking children ▫ Spanish-speaking children with SLI performed significantly worse than the typically developing Spanish-speaking children

Factors related to NWR performance • Possible potential prosodic influences on poor NWR • Hanson et al. (1999) ▫ Swedish children with SLI were six times more likely to omit unstressed syllables in weak-strong syllable combination than strong-weak syllable combination

Factors related to NWR performance • Linguistic experience ▫ L1 appears to influence NWR performance in L2 (Gutierrez-Clellen & Simon-Cerejido, 2010; Summers et al., 2009.

• Windsor, Kohnert, Lobitz, & Pham (2010) ▫ Investigated NWR between bilingual SpanishEnglish and monolingual English-speaking children with and without language impairment.

Previous studies • Windsor, Kohnert, Lobitz, & Pham (2010) ▫ Spanish-English bilinguals showed lower performance on English NWR tasks than English monolinguals ▫ NWR performance relies on native language experience and may be sensitive to language impairments.

Previous studies • Carter, Dillon, & Pisoni (2002) ▫ Examined prosodic characteristics of NWR task in children with cochlear implants ▫ Accuracy of number of syllables and primary stress ▫ 64% of NWR had the correct number of syllables ▫ 61% of NWR had the correct placement of primary stress.

Previous studies • Dillon, Clearly, Pisoni, & Carter (2004) ▫ 39% of consonants were produced correctly. ▫ Coronals were produced with greater accuracy than labials or dorsals. ▫ No difference across consonant manner or voicing.

Needs • Further insight into how linguistic background may influence NWR performance is needed. • Further investigation into prosody and production accuracy of consonant and vowel categories across various linguistic background is needed.

Purpose • To analyze the potential influence of various linguistic backgrounds on children’s English NWR accuracy. • To analyze segmental and supra-segmental accuracy of English NWR.

Relevance • Information obtained may prove useful for refining NWR measures to make them more appropriate for children from various linguistic backgrounds and to identify specific factors that influence their performance.

Methods • Participants ▫ ▫ ▫ ▫

15 Korean-English bilingual children (KE) 15 Chinese-English bilingual children (CE) 12 Spanish-English bilingual children (SE) 15 Monolingual English-speaking children (ME)

• No speech, language, hearing or other health problems

Bilingual children • All bilingual children acquired their home language first and learned English at Englishspeaking daycare, preschool, kindergarten. Onset of English language exposure

# of KEB # of CEB

# of SEB

Before 3

1

2

3

After 3-Before 5

7

8

3

After 5

7

5

6

Vocabulary and Phonological awareness 140

120

Standard Scores

100

80 PPVT

60

ELLA

40

20

0

ME

KEB

CEB

SEB

Consonants

Vowels

Stress-timed vs. Syllable-timed • Stress-timed: English ▫ Trochaic vs. iambic

• Syllable-timed: Spanish, Chinese, Korean

Dominant syllable types • English: mono-syllable • Korean: di-syllable • Chinese: mono-syllable • Spanish: multi-syllable

Data Collection • 64 Nonword repetition stimuli ▫ 2-3 syllable nonwords (Edwards et al., 2004) ▫ 4-5 syllable nonwords (Gathercole et al., 1994)

Edwards et al. (2004) • /vugim/ • /bɪdæg/ • /donug/ • /mɪnæmp/ • /kɛdəwəmb/ • /tikəpo/

Gathercole et al. (1994)

Data collection • The adopted nonwords were produced by two native English speakers • The recorded stimuli were played via computer • Each child was asked to repeat what he/she heard

Transcription • All responses were transcribed by native English speakers using IPA. • A second transcriber transcribed 20% of NWR ▫ Mean reliability for consonants = 89% ▫ Mean reliability for vowels = 87%

Data analysis • All data were entered into the LIPP (Oller & DeAngelo, 2001)

Data analysis: Segmental analysis • Percentage of consonants correct (PCC) ▫ (length of syllable * groups)

• Consonant accuracy: total & each syllable length • Percentage of vowels correct (PVC) ▫ (length of syllable * groups)

• Vowel accuracy: total & each syllable length

Data analysis: Supra-segmental analysis • Percentage of accuracy for primary stress • (length of syllable * groups)

NWR: Percentage of Consonant Correct 100

90

PCC

80

70

ME KEB

60

50

CEB SEB 2 syllables

3 syllables

4 syllables

5 syllables

Total

Consonant Manner

NWR: Percentage of Vowel Correct 100

90

PVC

80

70

ME KEB

60

50

CEB SEB 2 syllables

3 syllables

4 syllables

5 syllables

Front vowels

More lax vowel errors than tense or corner vowels KEB & CEB produced more errors than ME & SEB

Central vowels

No significant differences among groups

Back vowels

More lax vowel errors than tense and corner vowels

Supra-segmental analysis

2 Syllable NWR

3 Syllable NWR

KEB: more voicing errors

4 Syllable NWR

SEB: more place and manner errors than CEB and ME

5 Syllable NWR

SEB & KEB more errors than ME & CEB

2 Syllable NWR

3 Syllable NWR

4 Syllable NWR

5 Syllable NWR

2 Syllable NWR

3 Syllable NWR

CEB & KEB more vowel errors than SEB

4 Syllable NWR

No group differences

5 Syllable NWR

CEB & KEB more errors than ME & SEB

2 Syllable NWR

3 Syllable NWR

4 Syllable NWR

5 Syllable NWR

2 Syllable NWR

3 Syllable NWR

4 Syllable NWR

5 Syllable NWR

Discussion 1. Analyze performance according to consonants. •



Consonant accuracy: SE and KE groups displayed lower accuracy than monolingual and CE groups for 4 and 5 syllable NWR. English and Chinese contain larger consonantal inventories than Korean and Spanish. This may affect production accuracy of fricatives.

Discussion 2. Analyze performance according to vowels. • •

Vowel accuracy: CE and KE groups displayed lower accuracy than monolingual and SE groups. Consonant accuracy may yield less-linguistically biased results than vowels, on which greater differences emerged as word length increased.

Implications • Results provide further support for the view that, even in bilinguals with relatively strong English skills, NWR tasks do not eliminate the role of linguistic experience. • NWR tasks are often used to tap PSTM, yet there are clearly language-specific effects not only in terms of production accuracy, but also seemingly, on the underlying processes that influence performance.

Consonants in NWR • Consonants in NWR have been handled differently: • Dollaghan & Campbell ▫ Excluded late 8 sounds (sibilants & liquids)

• Gathercole & Baddeley ▫ Included late 8 sounds

Vowels in NWR • Vowels in NWR have been handled differently: • Dollaghan & Campbell ▫ tense vowels bc of increased perceptibility (and thus violate typical English metrical pattern which includes weak syllables)

• Gathercole & Baddeley ▫ both tense and lax vowels, and included weak syllables

Prosody in NWR • Prosody in NWR have been handled differently: • Dollaghan & Campbell ▫ Used only tense vowels, contrast with the typical English stress patterns.

• Gathercole & Baddeley ▫ Contained weak syllables with a reduced vowel, natural prosodic patterns of English

Smile Life

When life gives you a hundred reasons to cry, show life that you have a thousand reasons to smile

Get in touch

© Copyright 2015 - 2024 PDFFOX.COM - All rights reserved.