Morphological Types of Languages - UC Berkeley Linguistics [PDF]

Feb 26, 2016 - Some languages have vowel harmony that applies to entire words--- for example, in Turkish all the vowels

274 downloads 21 Views 1MB Size

Recommend Stories


UC Berkeley
It always seems impossible until it is done. Nelson Mandela

UC Berkeley
Just as there is no loss of basic energy in the universe, so no thought or action is without its effects,

UC Berkeley
What you seek is seeking you. Rumi

UC Berkeley
You have survived, EVERY SINGLE bad day so far. Anonymous

UC Berkeley
Goodbyes are only for those who love with their eyes. Because for those who love with heart and soul

UC Berkeley
Stop acting so small. You are the universe in ecstatic motion. Rumi

UC Berkeley
Raise your words, not voice. It is rain that grows flowers, not thunder. Rumi

UC Berkeley
The happiest people don't have the best of everything, they just make the best of everything. Anony

UC Berkeley
The greatest of richness is the richness of the soul. Prophet Muhammad (Peace be upon him)

UC Berkeley
Don't be satisfied with stories, how things have gone with others. Unfold your own myth. Rumi

Idea Transcript


Morphological Typology Ling 100 – Introduction to Linguistic Science Guest Lecture – Jonathan Manker 26 February 2016

What is Typology? • Linguistic typology is a branch of linguistics that attempts to categorize languages based on similarities in structure (phonological inventories, grammatical constructions, word order, etc.)

Typological Map of Consonant Inventory Sizes

Morphological Typology • Languages have a wide variety of morphological processes available (e.g. different types of affixation, etc.) for creating words and word forms.

• However, languages vary with respect to what morphological processes are available, how frequently they are used, and what types of information can be encoded in these processes. • In this lecture, we’ll look at differences in morphology among a variety of languages and learn to categorize these languages based on their morphological patterns.

What is a word? • Before we discuss properties of word structure in different languages, we have to define what it is we mean when we refer to something as a ‘word.’ • Question: Think about the two strings of sounds below. How many words are in each? [ðəkwɪkbɹaʊnfaksd͡ʒʌmpsoʊvɹ̩ðəleɪzidɔg] [æntaɪdɪsəstæblɪʃmɛnteɹiənɪzm̩]

• So what makes [ðəkwɪkbɹaʊnfaksd͡ʒʌmpsoʊvɹ̩ðəleɪzidɔg] a sentence, but [æntaɪdɪsəstæblɪʃmɛnteɹiənɪzm̩] a word? (Both have 7-8 morphemes) • There is no universal expectation for what words should be like in different languages. We will see examples in other languages that are structurally similar to the first, but are considered a single word.

The Phonological Word • Even for speakers of non-written languages there seems to be a concept of the word which is in some ways psychologically relevant. • These intuitions about what words are often coincide with the domain of certain phonological rules which are sensitive to word boundaries and the word as a unit of structure and organization. These are usually language specific.

• Stress. In English, each content word will have exactly one primary stress. Do the following examples all seem like single words? Where is their primary stress? • dehumidifier • recapitulation • antidisestablishmentarianism

Phonological Evidence for the word • Some languages have vowel harmony that applies to entire words--for example, in Turkish all the vowels in most words must be all front vowels or all back vowels. We never find vowel harmony occurring over entire sentences. • /el-ler-in/ ‘hand’-PLR-gen. vs. /at-lar-ɯn/ ‘horse,’-PLR-gen. • We’ve also discussed phonotactic considerations: certain sequences of sounds cannot occur within syllables, but may be permissible over word boundaries (e.g. [dzm], above in ‘words must’)

Morphological Evidence for the Word • Positional mobility --- word form as a whole can be moved. • Eg. I love plums, Plums I love. vs. I love dehumidifiers, de I love humidifiers. (!) • Uninterruptability --- extraneous material cannot be introduced into the middle of a word-form. • Eg. A dehumidifier, a, well, dehumidifier, a de, well, humidifer (!) • Internal stability --- fixed order of morphemes within word forms • Dehumidifiers, ifyshumiderde (!)

Morphological types across the world’s languages • Linguists can categorize languages based on their word-building properties and usage of different affixation processes. • The broadest distinction among languages is whether or not affixation is allowed at all, or if every word must be a single morpheme. • For languages that allow affixation, we can further categorize these according to their morphological characteristics

Analytic and Isolating Languages • Analytic languages have sentences composed entirely of free morphemes, where each word consists of only one morpheme. • Isolating languages are “purely analytic” and allow no affixation (inflectional or derivational) at all. Sometimes analytic languages allow some derivational morphology such as compounds (two free roots in a single word)

• A canonically analytic language is Mandarin Chinese. Note that properties such as “plural” and “past” comprise their own morphemes and their own words. • [wɔ mən tan tçin lə] • 1st PLR play piano PST • ‘we played the piano’

Synthetic Languages • Synthetic languages allow affixation such that words may (though are not required to) include two or more morphemes. These languages have bound morphemes, meaning they must be attached to another word (whereas analytic languages only have free morphemes). • Synthetic languages include three subcategories: agglutinative, fusional, and polysynthetic.

Synthetic Language Type 1: Agglutinative • Agglutinative languages have words which may consist of more than one, and possibly many, morphemes. • The key characteristic separating agglutinative languages from other synthetic languages is that morphemes within words are easily parsed or “loosely” arranged; the morpheme boundaries are easy to identify. 1:many word to morpheme ratio; 1:1 morpheme to meaning • We often use the metaphor “beads on a string” to describe agglutinative languages.

Agglutinative languages • Examples of canonical agglutinative languages include Turkish, Swahili, Hungarian • el-ler-imiz-in (Turkish) • hand-plr.-1st plr.-genitive case, ‘of our hands’ • ni-na-soma (Swahili) • I-present-read ‘I am reading’ • (also u-na-soma ‘you read,’ ni-li-soma ‘I read,’ etc.)

Synthetic Language Type 2: Fusional • Fusional languages, like other synthetic languages, may have more than one morpheme per word • However, fusional languages may have morphemes that combine multiple pieces of grammatical information; that is, there is not a clear 1 to 1 relationship between grammatical information and morphemes • For example, in Spanish: • [ˈabl-o] ‘I am speaking’ -[o] suffix means 1st person sng., present tense • [ˈabl-a] ‘s/he is speaking’ -[a] suffix means 3rd person sng. present tense • [abl-ˈo] ‘s/he spoke’ -[ˈo] suffix with stress means 3rd singular past tense

Fusional Languages • Latin fusion: [re:ksisti] ‘you all ruled’ • There are four pieces of grammatical information and four morphs, however the ‘perfective’ meaning is shared among several morphs.

Synthetic Language Type 3: Polysynthetic • Polysynthetic languages often display a high degree of affixation (high number of morphemes per word) and fusion of morphemes, like agglutinative and fusional languages. • Additionally, however, polysynthetic languages may have words with multiple stems in a single word (which are not compounds). This may be achieved by incorporating the subject and object nouns into complex verb forms.

• • • •

For example: anin- ɲam-jɔ-te-n (Sora) he-catch-fish-nonpast-do ‘he is fish-catching’ This is called noun incorporation, where the object ‘fish’ is incorporated in the verb ‘catch.’

Polysynthetic Languages • Some of the most extreme examples come from Eskimo languages such as West Greenlandic: • tusaa-nngit-su-usaar-tuaannar-sinnaa-nngi-vip-putit • ‘hear’-neg.-intrans.participle-‘pretend’-‘all the time’-‘can’-neg.‘really’-2nd.sng.indicative • ‘You simply cannot pretend not to be hearing all the time’

But in reality… • So we’ve looked at canonical examples of four types of languages: analytical, agglutinative, fusional, and polysynthetic. • But languages often show elements of different morphological types. • If a language is hard to classify as one of the four main types, it may be considered “mixed.” The properties that distinguish these types may in fact be gradient rather than categorical.

Classifying languages into morphological types • Ask yourself the following questions: • 1) How many morphemes can occur in a single word?

• If the answer is one, or usually one, the language is analytical. Otherwise, it is probably synthetic. A language with a few might be fusional, agglutinative, or polysynthetic; A language with many is probably agglutinative or polysynthetic (since fusional morphemes may contain multiple bits of grammatical information).

Classifying languages into morphological types • 2) If the language allows affixation, are the morphemes easy to divide? Is each piece of grammatical information contained in a single morpheme (and the reverse)?

Classifying languages into morphological types • 3) Does the language allow words with multiple roots (such as noun incorporation)? If yes, the language is likely polysynthetic. • These languages may also have a high degree of fusion and may contain many morphemes in one word (see Greenlandic example) .

Practice – categorize the language • Ancient Greek • [lu-o:] ‘I release’ • release-1st person singular present active indicative • [lu-e:] ‘You should release’ • release-2nd person singular present middle subjunctive • [lu:-etai] ‘he is being released’ • release-3rd person singular present passive indicative

• Ancient Greek- fusional! • Each word contains only two morphemes (so it’s not analytical), but the suffix contains information about person, number, tense, mood, and voice

Practice – categorize the language • Aztecan • [ni-ki-ta] ‘I see it’ • I-it-see • [ni-ki-ta-k] ‘I saw it’ • I-it-see-past

• Aztecan - Agglutinative! • Morphemes each contain one bit of information and are easily divisible

Practice – categorize the language • English • ‘The boy will play with the dog.’ • ‘John’s cat eats mice.’ • ‘antidisestablishmentarianism’

• English – mixed • The first example is mostly analytical • The second demonstrates some fusional properties • The third shows a word that seems agglutinative • Generally speaking, English has very little inflection left and has been becoming increasingly analytical, although words may contain many derivational affixes.

Practice – categorize the language • Han (Athabascan)

‘we’re starting to work’

‘we will hookcatch (fish)’

• Han – polysynthetic! (noun incorporation. Also a high degree of fusion) • (Although interestingly enough, while verbs tend to be polysynthetic, nouns are almost analytical and can only have a single possessive prefix--- no case, number, gender, definiteness, etc. is indicated).

Typological Overview of the World’s Languages – WALS maps • Looking across the world’s languages, we see groups of languages in common geographical regions that have similar morphological characteristics.

Inflectional synthesis of the verb

(modern day languages)

• Is this evidence of languages sharing a language family? • Sometimes, but not always. Language families are defined by having a genetic relationship, where elements of the language are passed down (through time). • Sometimes features, even morphological characteristics, can spread among languages that are spoken near each other, even when unrelated. We call these areal features (e.g., see southeast Asia, N. America).

Smile Life

When life gives you a hundred reasons to cry, show life that you have a thousand reasons to smile

Get in touch

© Copyright 2015 - 2024 PDFFOX.COM - All rights reserved.