Research Article - Stanford University [PDF]

Research Article. CRITICAL EVIDENCE: A Test of the Critical-Period Hypothesis for. Second-Language Acquisition. Kenji Ha

208 downloads 50 Views 600KB Size

Recommend Stories


Stanford University
Pretending to not be afraid is as good as actually not being afraid. David Letterman

Danny Stockli, Stanford University, Stanford
At the end of your life, you will never regret not having passed one more test, not winning one more

1994 Publications Summary of the Stanford ... - Stanford University [PDF]
Jan 1, 1995 - actions with individual deadlines (overhead would be too great), or as a single transaction (it is a continuous ..... Our study raised the following issues as hindrances in the applicability of such systems. workflow ...... are willing

Stanford University Investment Report
Don't be satisfied with stories, how things have gone with others. Unfold your own myth. Rumi

MS, Stanford University
Ask yourself: What would I be risking if I did some of the things that are outside of my comfort zone?

Varieties of situation semantics - Stanford University [PDF]
Apr 11, 2013 - 1 Initial motivation. 1. 2 Possibilistic situation semantics. 3. 3 Relation to information-based situation semantics. 6. 4 Relation to event semantics.

stanford university neuroradiology fellowship
Your big opportunity may be right where you are now. Napoleon Hill

RESEARCH ARTICLE [PDF]
May 31, 2016 - oral hygiene was difficult because of the concave pontic design. The sanitary pontic and the modified ridge lap pontic were developed to avoid or minimize any contact between the pontic and edentulous ridge mucosa, but they did not sat

RESEARCH ARTICLE [PDF]
Jul 31, 2017 - curriculum implementation in schools. Hence, the researcher used the Tyl. (1950) to evaluate the implementation of Primary School Standard Curr. Kurikulum Standard Sekolah Rendah (KSSR) of the subject World of Sci. (WST). Based on Tyle

Untitled - CS230 - Stanford University
Knock, And He'll open the door. Vanish, And He'll make you shine like the sun. Fall, And He'll raise

Idea Transcript


PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Research Article CRITICAL EVIDENCE: A Test of the Critical-Period Hypothesis for Second-Language Acquisition Kenji Hakuta,' Ellen Bialystok,2 and Edward Wiley' 'Stanford-Universityand 'York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Abstract-me crirical-period hypothesisfor second-language acquisition was rested on doto from the 1990 U.S. Census using responses from 2.3 million immigrants with Spanish or Chinese language backgrounds. The analyses rested a key prediction of the hypothesis, namely, that the line regressing second-language attainment on age of immigration would be markedly different on either side ofrhe criticalage point. Predictions tested were that there would be a difference in slope, a difference in the mean while controlling f o r slope, o r both. The results showed large linear effectsfor level ofeducation and f o r age of immigration, but a negligible amount of additional variance was accounted f o r when the parameters f o r diffeerence in slope and difference in means were estimated. Thus, the pattern of decline in second-language acquisitionfailed to produce the discontinuity that is an essential hallmark o f a critical period.

and Newport (1989, 1991) have wed,for example, that the= is a strong age-related decline in proficiency for languages learned prior to pubetty (definedas 15yean old) and random variation in achievement among individuals who are exposed to a second language later in life. Such develop mental discontinuity at an identifiable mamtional time would constitute suppon for the two conditions of a critical period. The data, however, are controversial because of the difficulty in separating out the effects of age of initial exposure, duration of exposure, and social and linguistic backgrounds of the pdcipants (see the analysis and critique of Johnson and Newport's study in Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994). Other researchers have argued that the evidence fails to support the interpretation that language-learning potential is fundamentally changed after a critical period (e&, Epstein, Flynn, & Mdohardjono, 1996; Hakuq 2001). Two kinds of evidence have typically been used in these challenges. The first is the identification of older learners who achieve nativelike competence in the second language (Birdsong, 1992; Bongaem, Planken, & The idea that there is a biologically based critical period for secondSchils, 1995; Ioup, Boustagui, El Tigi, & Moselle, 1994). The second is language acquisition that prevents older learners from achieving nativebehavioral evidence that fails to revcal a qualitative change in learning outLike competence has appeal lo both theorists and social policymakers comes at the close of a critical period (Bialystok & Haku4 1999; Bialy(Bailey, Brner, Symons, & Lichtman, 2001). The critical-period hypothesis was originally proposed in the neurolinguistic litemture by Penfield stok & Miller, 1999; Birdsong & Flege, 2ooO; Birdsong & Molis, 2001; Flege. 1999; Flege. Munro, & MacKay, 1995; Flege et al., 1999). Whether and Robem (1959) and vigorously followed up by Lenneberg (1967). such evidence is considered damaging to the critical-periodhypothesi dewho speculated that maturational aspects of the brain that limited recovpends on the stringency of the criteria for defining the boundaries of the ery from brain traumas and disorders would extend to second-language critical period (Birdsong, 1999; Harley & Wang, 1997; Singleton & acquisition. Subsequent research using behavioral evidence appeared Lengyel, 1995). Nonetheless, both weak and smng interpretationsof the to confirm this hypothesis (Johnson, 1992; Johnson & NewpoIf 1989; critical-period hypothesis require the demonstrationof a significant change Oyama, 1976; Patkowski, 1980, 1994). The measure of language pmfiin learning outcome, not merely a monotonic decline with age. ciency in these studies varied (ratings of oral speech, grammaticalityjudgDefense of the position that language learning is constrained by a critiment tasks), but the typical result was that proficiency scores declined cal period requires specifying the maturational stage at which languagewith increases in age of initial exposure to the second language. learning potential changes, and ideally the reason for the change. However, The claim that there is an age-related decline in the success with there has been little consensus about what age constitutesthe critical point, which individuals master a second language is not controversial. The and m o n s for pmposing di8erent ages have rarely been offered. Rediminished average achievement of older learners is supported by persearchers have variously claimed, for example, that the age at which the sonal anecdote and documented by empirical evidence (Flege, Yenicritical period terminates is 5 years w h e n , 1973),6 years (Pinker, 1994). Komshian, & Liu, 1999; Stevens, 1999).What is controversial, though, is 12 years @meberg, 1967). or 15 years (Johnson & Newport, 1989). whether this pattern meets the conditions for concluding that a critical An alternative to the critical-period hypothesis is that second-language period constrains learning in a way predicted by the theory. A critical leaming becomes compromised with age, potentially because of factors period minimally entails two characteristics: (a) a high level of preparedness for learning within a specified developmental period to en- that are not specific to language but nevertheless interfere with the individual's ability to leam a new language. 'These might include social and edusure the domain is mastered by the species and @) a lack of preparedness cational variables that influence learning potential and oppom~Nty,as well outside this period (Bornstein, 1989; Colombo, 1982). The consequence of these conditions is that the relation between learning and as cognitive aging that gradually erodes some of the mechanisms necessary for learning a complex body of knowledge, such as a new language. age is different inside and outside the critical period. Among social factors, education has been most clearly demonstrated hponents of a critical-period explanation have attempted to place the to influence second-language acquisition. Leamers who anive as Wdescription of second-languagelearning within these pammeters. Johnson grants at different ages have fundamentally different experiences, are exposed to qualitatively and quantitatively different samples of the new language, and have distinctly different opportunities for formal study of Address correspondence to Kenji Hakula, CERAS Building, Stanford Uni- the language either directly or through other educational content (Bialyversity, Stanford, CA 94305: e-mail: [email protected]. stok & Hakuta, 1994; Flege et al., 1999). Flege and his colleagues havereVOL. 14, NO. 1, JANUARY 2003

Copyright 0 2003 Amencan Psychological Society

31

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Critical Period i n Second-Language Acquisition

Table 1. Regression of English pmfrciency on educarion: Spanish- and Chinese-speaking immigrants Variable

Parameter estimate

Intercept 5-8 years education Some high school High school graduate Some college

1.7431 0.2493 0.7324 1.0693 1.7398

Intercept 5-8 years education Some high school High school graduate Some college

2.0573 0.3484 0.8710 1.1708 1.4445

ss

SE Chinese 0.00417 0.00624 0.00586 0.00548 0.00451 Spanish 0.00136

0.00184 0.00196 0.00209 0.00198

F

96,590 884 8,659 21,071 82,450

174,334.0 1,596.0 15,628. I 38,030.6 148,813.0

1,796,840 28,171 154,633 244,933 417,988

2,293.71 1.0 35,961.6 197,393.0 312,664.0 533,572.0

P

Smile Life

When life gives you a hundred reasons to cry, show life that you have a thousand reasons to smile

Get in touch

© Copyright 2015 - 2024 PDFFOX.COM - All rights reserved.