THE ACQUISITION OF SECOND LANGUAGE ... - Jim Flege [PDF]

(Community. Language Learning) (Curran 1976), The Dartmouth ... through all of the above mentioned language teaching ...

0 downloads 10 Views 4MB Size

Recommend Stories


Benefits of Second Language Acquisition
The only limits you see are the ones you impose on yourself. Dr. Wayne Dyer

The Cambridge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition
Never wish them pain. That's not who you are. If they caused you pain, they must have pain inside. Wish

PDF Download Introducing Second Language Acquisition
Life isn't about getting and having, it's about giving and being. Kevin Kruse

PDF Read Introducing Second Language Acquisition
You often feel tired, not because you've done too much, but because you've done too little of what sparks

Second Language Acquisition and Autism
And you? When will you begin that long journey into yourself? Rumi

Second Language versus Heritage Language Acquisition
You often feel tired, not because you've done too much, but because you've done too little of what sparks

Second Language Acquisition of Focus Prosody
You have to expect things of yourself before you can do them. Michael Jordan

factors affecting the acquisition of english as a second language
The butterfly counts not months but moments, and has time enough. Rabindranath Tagore

Second Language Acquisition Gham'a-lah VS English
I cannot do all the good that the world needs, but the world needs all the good that I can do. Jana

Introduction to Instructed Second Language Acquisition
If you feel beautiful, then you are. Even if you don't, you still are. Terri Guillemets

Idea Transcript


THE ACQUISITION OF SECOND LANGUAGE PHONOLOGICAL SYSTEMS IN A COMMUNICATIVE FRAMEWORK - THE ROLE OF ATTITUDES AND EXPERIENCE Robert M. Hammond Purdue University

:ni

IVI

UniversityJames of Alabam}\•.• E. FI:g irmingham O. INTRODUCTION. Since researchers in second language acquisition and classroom language teachcrs both began to become disenchanted in the 1960's and 1970's with the Audio Lingual approach to second language instruction, many new ways and means of teaching second and/or foreign languages have been introduced. A partial list of major post-ALM models would include The Silent Way (Gattegno 1972 and 1976), Total Physical Response (Asher 1982), Counseling-Learning (Community Language Learning) (Curran 1976), The Dartmouth Intensive Language Model (DILM, a.k.a. The Rassias Method) (Rassias 1967), Suggestopedia (Suggestology) (Lozanov 1979), The Proficiency Movement (Omaggio 1986; James 1985; Byrnes and Canale 1987; Higgs 1984) and The Communicative Approach (Krashen and Terrell 1983; Widdowson 1978; Brumfit and Johnson 1982; Blair 1982; Johnson 1982; Oller and Richard-Amato 1983; Savignion and Berns 1984). Several of these models have, in turn, produced one or more spin-offs, and presentday second language teachers have a relatively large repertoire of classroom methodologies from which to choose.

The majority of these second language teaching methodologies have been strongly influenced by models of both Cognitive Psychology (Ausubel 1968: Anderson and Ausube11965) and Humanistic Psychology (Rogers 1951 and 1961). Likewise, most of the above-mentioned classroom methods, either directly or indirectly, can readily be classified as communicative approaches to second language acquisition. All versions of communcatively-based language teaching models have several major points of agreement: (1) they stress the meaningful use of a second language for the purposes of true communication in the classroom; (2) they require the presence of a maximally high amount of what Krashen (1981) calls comprehensible input, and; (3) again in Krashen's terms, these models stress the creation of a classroom environment which produces a maximally low affective filter. However, another common thread running through all of the above mentioned language teaching methodologies, including those that are not communicatively-based, is the fact that none of these current models makes any genuine effort to deal with the teaching of pronunciation in the second language classroom. It is particularly curious that propOnents of the so·called Proficiency Movement, while placing a great deal of emphasis on linguistic accuracy in the nascent stages of second language acquisition (to avoid what they

call fossilization) include no provision for the teaching of pronunciation in the classroom (See, for example, Omaggio 1986.). After surveying different varieties of communicati ve methodologies, Terrell (1989: 197) concludes that "Communicative approaches likewise have not known what to do with pronunciation". While none of the above-mentioned approach('~ to language teaching explicitly state that pronunciation is not to be taught, they all largely imply this by not including any type of pronunciation drills, explanations, exercises or any other explicit pronunciation instruction in their methodologies. The question that must be asked at this point, then. is why none of these post-ALM language teaching methodologies has chosen to include the teaching of the sound systems of second languages, since, after all, spoken language is the association of sound with meaning. There seem to be three principal reasons why methodologists have chosen not to include the teaching of pronunciation in current methodologies: (1) the explicit teaching of pronunciation appeals only to learning and not to acquisition, and is therefore of no value in a system that is attempting to get students to acquire language: (2) the constant reference to correct pronunciation or the correction of studcnt pronunciation errors witt inhibit a student from speaking by raising hislher affective filter. and; (3) since most second language instruction in the United States involves learners who have passed the socalled "ideal age" for language acquisition, it is felt that these adult students have already lost much of their innate capacity to acquire a native-like pronunciation in a second language. Because of the issue raised by the last of the above three points, the research in this paper was undertaken to attempt to assess some of the perception and imitation abilities of adult second language learners. We attempted to look at a number of factors through the use of a mimicry paradigm which tested subjects' abilities to detect and imitate a foreign accent Probably everyone has, at one time or another, heard speech produced with foreign accent. Previous research has established-not surprisingly-the fact that listeners arc able to detect foreign accent (Barry 1974), but apparently little is known about just what constitutes a foreign accenL There are, of course, measureable physical differences between the speech of native and non-native speakers which can be revealed by instrumental analysis (see, e.g. Suomi 1976; Flege 1980; Flege and Eefling 1987). The principal focus of the present study, however,

will be to report some of the effects of attitude experience on an individual's ability to perceive imitate a foreign accent.

and and

1. METHODOLOGY. BeforeJctually beginning the data-collection process. a pilothtudy was carried out to provide some idea of the kind of sound substitutions which occur in Spanish-accented English. Also. it was / hoped that this pilothludy would reveal the kinds of substitutions Americans might produce when trying to imitate a Spanish accent. Ten English sentences which included a large variety of English sounds which might ~ likely be mispronounced by native speakers of Spanish were recorded as spoken by native speakers of both Spanish and English. Based on these tapes. six English· sounds likely to be replaced by other sounds in Spanish--accented English were then chosen (See Table I). ~ A Spanish accent variant-the sound which seemed most likely to replace the target English sound in Spanish-accented English-was identified for each of these six English target sounds. All six of these expected Spanish-accent variants are sounds which occur in English in these same phonological environments. The presence of these sounds in Spanish-accented English can be readily understood in terms of differences in the sound systems of Spanish and English (Stockwell and Bowen 1965; Dalbor 1980).1 The six English target sounds shown in Table 1 were chosen to represent different segment types and positions ~ within the word: two of these were word-initial /

C)

~

consonants, two were word-final consonants, and !WOwere howels.2 Each of th# six English target sounds occurred

~

4-

were embedded both NP slots of the carrier 1) which in three different in Enghsh C(C)VC words (Table sentence

~ ~

is on the I This "The / yielded a total of 18 test sentences, each containing two

?

YI

Y, ."

different English target sounds of interest, and allowed a maximum of 36 possible Spanish-accent variants by each of our subjects. Subjects for this study were undergraduate American university students in four first-semester Spanish language classes. These students were chosen as subjects because their Spanish instructors were native speakers of Spanish

who speak English with what could be described as a heavy Spanish accenl. The experiment was carried out in a language laboratory where the test material was recorded On the same type of Wollensak tape recorders through head-set microphones held at a fixed distance from the mouth. Before recording the tcst material, each subject filled out an anonymous questionnaire which included questions concerning personal history (age, sex, place of birth, residence, etc.) and twelve questions designed to provide information concerning a subject's attitudes toward and exposure to foreign accent in general, and to Spanishaccented English in particular (Appendix I). The subjects were asked to read the English test sentences with the 'best imitation of a Spanish accent' they were capable of. They were not given explicit instructions concerning how to produce a Spanish accent. except that they were not to attempt to produce the impression of accentedness by pausing or stumbling. Subjects were asked to read each sentence twice and were allowed to repeat a sentence if they were not satisfied with their production. To avoid the possibility that subjects seated in adjacent booths might influence one another, two different randomizations of the eighteen test sentences were distributed. e.. Subject selection criteria were developed to,Ansure a homogeneous as possible population of subjects who were familiar with Spanish-accented English. From the original population of 137, subjects were eliminated from Ute study if: (I) their recording did not meet a minimal standard of sound quality; (2) they were not between the ages of 18 and 30;(3) they were not monolingual native speakers of English; or (4) they had not lived in the South Florida area of the United States for at least the last previous five years (an area in which a large number of native speakers of Caribbean Spanish reside). In addition, subjects were eliminated if, according to their own selfreport (Questions I, 3, and 12 on the questionnaire; see Appendix I) they knew no foreigners or had never heard a foreign accent. From the remaining population, 25 male and 25 female subjects were randomly selected. The 1800 English target sounds produced by the 50 subjects were then judged independently by two phonetically-trained listeners as falling into one of the following three discrete categories:

1In Spanish there are no lax vowels such as English III and /U/. orthographic "v" is produced phonetically as either [bl or

[~I.depending on the phonological environment. and [zl does not occur either intervocalically or in utterance-final position. Furthermore. in Caribbean Spanish dialects. [e

English (e.g., vice [vays]).· J 1. The target sound was produced as it normal1y occurs in)

and [§] may be in Cree-variation as surface realizations of !CI.

accent variant (e.g., vice [bays]). ) 3. The target sound was replaced by some other than the expected Spanish-accent variant (e.g., vice [mays]).

I

and final In/ may be manifested phonetically as [Q]. 20ne additional English target sound. Idi. was included in the test material for instrumental analysis. It was not included in the present study because the pilot-study reyealcd that while Spanish speakers of English are apt to deaspirate English It/. their production of it will probably not be heard as English Id/ by American listeners.

2. The target sound was replaced by the expected Spanish-

The two judges agreed initially in 91.2% of cases. The 40 sounds (2.2% of cases) for which agreement could not be reached after further listening were submitted to a third

more contact our subjects had with foreigners in general,

phonelically-lrained judge. To be scored as having produced the Spanish-accent variant a subject need have produced it only once during each of the two productions of a leStsentence. Differences in the frequency of production of Englishaccent variants were analyzed according to the independent variables of informant sex, phoneme identity, segment type, and position in the word and sentence. The significance of these differences were then tested by Chisquare analyses. A number of seales were also constructed from responses to the questionnaire, and the values of these scales were examined by a Spearman rank-order correlation analysis to determine if they would correlate with the total number of Spanish-accent variants produced by subjects. Our subjects produced a total of 508 Spanish-accent variants, averaging about 10 per subject and ranging between 0 and 27 different variants per subject There was no statistically significant difference between male and female subjects in frequency of production of these Spanish-accent variants. In the total corpus of 508 variants, only 11 'other' variants (Le., non-'S pan ishaccent' variants) were produced.



of Spanish-accent variants they produced. But at the same time, we found that subjects' attitude toward foreign accent was inverselr correlated with the number of Spanishaccent variants produced. That is, the more negative a according to their own self-report, the greater the number ~ subject's attitude was toward foreign accent, the greater

"

2.0. EFFECTS OF AITITUDE AND EXPERIENCE. For the present study, we were interested only in determining whether familiarity with, or attitude toward foreign accent would be related to subjects' ability to imitate a Spanish accent. A number of scales were constructed from responses to the questionnaire which subjects completed before beginning the imitation experiment (Appendix 1). The first scale concerned each subject's familiarity with Spanish accent and the second scale dealt with subjects' attitude toward Spanish accent. The third and fourth scales concerned a subject's attitude toward and familiarity with foreign accent in general. A Spearman rank-order correlation analysis was performed to determine if there was a relationship between ratings on these four scales and the total number of Spanish accent

~ ~ts produced by each subject , 2.1 FINDINGS. We found that neither subjects' familiarity specifically with Spanish-accented English (Scale I, Table 2) nor subjects' attitude toward Spanishaccented English (Scale II, Table 2) correlated with the number of Spanish-accent variants produced by subjects} However, we did find significant correlations between speakers' familiarity with foreigners (Scale III, Table 2) and speaker attitude toward foreign accent in general (Scale IV, Table 2). Subjects' familiarity with foreign accent in general was positively correlated with the number of Spanish-accent variants produced (p1\05). That is, the "

0

3In the construction of this scale. one point was added to the response given to Question 6 (where the leftmost column was scored "1" and the rightmost column scored "4" if "Spanish" was the response to Question 7: one point was sublraCled if "Spanish" was the response to Question 11.

~II

waj the number of Spanish-accent variants slbe produced (p"OI). 2.2 DISCUSSION. These results seem to indicate that there is indeed a relationship between a person's attitude toward foreign accent and his/her ability to imitate one. It is somewhat surprising that neither subjects' familiarity with, nor attitude specifically toward Spanish-accented English seemed to affect our subjects' abilities to articulate relevant sound substitutions in their attempts to produce Spanish-accented English. We expected that a speaker's ability to produce some of the sound substitutions which are characteristic of Spanish-accented English would have depended to some extent either on how often the subject hears Spanish-accented English, or

~

how slbe speak feels either .mut is Hispanics or the about how Hispanics Englis".Jt1t possible that subjects' self-reports were not accurate because questions touching on these factors were perhaps too direct, especially those regarding attitude toward Spanish accent and particularly in light of the potential social bond formed between (some of) the students and their Hispanic teacher. We did, however, find a slight tendency for greater contact with Spanish accent to correspond to a better imitation of Spanish accent, but this correlation was not statistically significant. On the basis of our present preliminary findings, we can probably maintain, at least tentatively, that imitating a foreign accent represents a kind of skill. Spanish accent is, of course, a form of foreign accent, and we did find that greater familiarity with foreign accent in general correlated with ability to imitate Spanish accent. It mayor may not be the case that a subject's experience hearing a Hungarian accent in English, for example, would help that person imitate Spanish-accented English, since it is possible that overall experience with foreign accents in general may make a listener more acutely aware of an additional foreign accent in hislber native language, and may likewise help this same individual in the task of imitating foreign accent. We can, however, probably assume that familiarity with Spanish accent has increased the subjects' . ability to imitate a Spanish accent in English, and that the questionnaire was not sufficiently sensitive in assessing subjects' familiarity with Spanish-accented English. Initially, the finding that subjects having a negative attitude toward a foreign accenttendcd to be better able to imitate a Spanish accent than subjects with a positive attitude might appear to be counter-intuitive if one assumes that dislike or disapproval of a group will lead speakers to pay less attention to the speech of that group. However, the reverse may aClually be true if we assume

Itf.,

that the behavior tapped by this experiment is indeed a skill. The more negative an individual feels toward foreign accent, the more closely s/he may attend to it. Social psychologists discuss a phenomenon known as 'sensitization' which seems to apply to our present findings. For example, Himmelfarb (1966) reports that antisemites seem to be better able than unprejudiced individuals to identify photos of Jews. Along this same line, the close attention that members of one social, religious or racial group pay to the characteristics of another group often serves as the source of ethnic jokes, nicknames, and even fossilized and now-accepted expressions, e.g. "to get off Scot-free". Furthermore, some people specifically utilize imitations of foreign accent to deride other national or ethnic groups. This last observation suggests the possibility that persons having a very negative attitude toward accentedness may actually acquire their skill at imitating through practice. Also, a common subjective experience is for the foreign accent of an acquaintance to seem to disappear, or at least we seem to become less overtly aware of it, as we develop a friendship or social bond with that person. An alternative interpretation to the present findings might be that some individuals develop a negative attitude toward accentedness because they are relatively more sensitive to phonetic differences than are other people. However, in the absence of any evidence that some individuals have this kind of hyper-sensitivity, we can probably maintain that differences in altitude toward accentedness may indeed lead to differences in ability to imitate foreign accent. 3. CONCLUSIONS. It seems that the ability to imitate foreign accent depends to a certain extent on exposure to foreign accent., but that a person's attitudes may be even more important, a negative attitude causing a speaker to attend more closely to phonetic differences between native and non-native speakers. Furthermore, the correlation which was found in the present study between attitudes and familiarity with foreign accent with subjects' ability to imitate foreign aceent suggests that an imitation task such as the present one may prove to be a useful indirect method of assessing attitudes toward linguistic differences. . A preliminary examination of other data provided by the mimicry paradigm used in the present study suggests that listeners may be more sensitive to misarticulations which occur in certain positions within a word and sentence than they are to others. Obviously, a follow-up study which includes a larger corpus of data must be carried out before such a hypothesis can be maintained. Also, these data suggest that the imitation of foreign accent reflects a set of internal stereotypes which develop as a result of hearing a foreign accent, in light of the fact that (1) subjects in this study were able to produce many sound substitutions which actually occur in Spanish-accented English; and (2) they produced more of certain sound substitutions than others. Given the fnct that nil of the Spanish-accent

variants produced in this study are sounds which occur in English, a preliminary hypothesis that perception of foreign accent is based at least in part on internal stereotypes, and not wholly on experience, appears to be supported. Finally, with respect to the question of whether adult language learners arc able to perceive and produce sounds they hear, the data herein and in other research (see for example Flege and Hammond 1982) strongly suggest that at least many adult language learners have not lost the ability to hear many subtle phonetic differences, and in turn, under certain conditions, to articulate such differences. What this information ultimately tells us about the acquisition of second language sound systems by adults, or how the teaching of pronunciation should fit into a communicative language teaching methodology is still an open question and must await the results of further research. It seems clear, however, that adult second language learners are capable of far greater powers of perception and sound production than they have been given credit for by many second language researchers.

Table 1

und

Variant Isl 171 Inl [i)III Ivl Spanish-accent ~~ ~sJlJ) [b) ~~ English [e) Target ~ Is)

j\[U)

LcJtical Forms

fig, pig, wig book, hook, crook vice, veil, vISe shell, sheet, sheep nose, cheese, hose bean, phone, bone ••

n

Table 2 Scale 1: ~cale

ll:

~cale

ill:

}~cale

IV:

Familiarity with Spanish accent. Questions 10, 12. Range: 3-8 Attitude toward Spanish accent. Questions 6, 7, 11. Range: 0-5 Familiarity with Foreign accent. Questions 1,3. Range: 4·8 Attitude toward Foreign accent. Questions 2, 4, 5, 8. Range: 4·16

-0.114 ":1

.0.Osfl () .284tf s: -0.37~

It.

0.429 0.550 0.045

0.007

Appendix I: Foreign Accent Survey Speaker number Age Sex Home How long

_ _ _

Place of birth Mother tongue (native language) Foreign language(s) spoken fluently _,

Cheek only one: 1. How many foreigners do you \cnow? ~--.-NONE -----AFEW __ try MANY . If you were a foreigner, would you to get rid of__yourVERY accent?MANY YES PROBABLY MAYBE NO ~ How many ~ple do you 1cnowthat speak with a foreign accent? -----..NONE -----A FEW __ MANY __ VERY MANY 4. Foreign accents sound __ VERY BAD __ BAD __ OK __ NICE S. A foreign ac:c:entwiI\ hurt a person's chances for success. __ YES __ PROBABLY __ MAYBE ~O 6, A Spanish accent sounds __ VERY BAD __ BAD __ OK ~CE 7. Which accent do you like most? __ FRENCH __ RNNISH __ SPANISH __ GERMAN 8. If you lived in Europe, would you try to lose your accent? __ NO __ MAYBE __ PROBABLY __ YES 9. Intelligent, hard· working people can always lose their foreign accen!. --.-NO __ MAYBE __ PROBABLY __ YES 10. How often do you talk to people with a Spanish accent? __ VERY OfTEN __ OfTEN __ SELDOM __ NEVER 11. Which accent do you like the least? __ FINNISH __ GERMAN __ SPANISH __ FRENCH 12. How much do you hear a Spanish accent? __ NEVER __ SELDOM __ OfTEN __ VERY OfTEN

_ _

References Ausubel, David. 1968. Educational psychology - a cognitive view. New York: Holl, Rinehart and Winston. Anderson, Richard C. and David Ausubel (cds.). 1965. Readings in the psychology of cognition. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Asher, James J. 1982. Learning another language through actions - the complete teacher's guidebook. 2nd ed. Los Gatos, CA: Sky Oaks Productions. Barry, W. 1974. Language background and the perception of foreign accent Journal of Phonetics 2.6589. Bjarkman, Peter C. and Robert M. Hammond (cds.). 1989. American Spanish pronunciation - theoretical and applied perspectives. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Blair, R. 1982. Innovative approaches to language leaChing.Rowley, MA: Newbury House. Brumfit, Christopher J. and Keith Johnson. 1979. The communicative approach to language teaching. New York: Oxford University Press. Byrnes, H. and Michael Canale (cds.). 1987. Defining and developing proficiency, Lincolnwood, ll.: National Textbook Company. Curran, Charles A. 1976. Counseling-learning in second languages. Apple River, ll.: Apple River Press. Dalbor, John. 1980. Spanish phonology: theory and practice. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Finocchiaro, Mary, and Christopher J. Brumfit. 1983. The functional-notional approach. New York: Oxford University Press. FIege, James E. 1980. Phonetic approximation in second language acquisition. Language Learning 30.117-134.

FIege, James E. and Wieke Eefting. 1987. Production and perception of English stops by native Spanish speakers. Journal of Phonetics 15.67-83. Flege James E. and Robert M. Hammond. 1982. Mimicry of non-distinctive phonetic differences between language varieties. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 5-1.1-17. Gattegno, Caleb. 1972. Teaching foreign languages in schools: the silent way. 2nd cd. New York: Educational Solutions Inc. ___ , 1976. The common sense of teaching foreign languages. New York: Educational Solutions Inc. Hammond, Robert M. 1986. Error analysis and the natural approach to teaching foreign languages. Lenguas Modemas 13.129-39. 1988. Accuracy versus communicative competency: The acquisition of grammar in the second language classroom. J lispan;a 71.408-17. ___ . In press. Pronunciation proficiency and the affective filter - attitudes and language acquisition. In: Language Proficiency: Defining, teaching, testing, cd.

by L. Arena. New York: Plenum Publishing Company. (to appear 1989) Higgs, Theodore V. (cd.). 1984. Teaching for proficiency: The organizing principle. Lincolnwood, IL: National Textbook Company. Himmelfarb, S. 1966. Studies in the perception of ethnic group membership: accuracy, response bias, and anti-semitism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 4.347-355. loup, Georgette, and Steven Weinberger (eds.). 1987. Intcrlanguage phonology: The acquisition of a second language sound system. Cambridge, MA: Newbury House. James, Carl (cd.). 1985. Foreign language proficiency in the classroom and beyond. Lincolnwood, IL: National Textbook Company. Johnson, Keith. 1982. Communicative syllabus d:-sign and methodology. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Krashen, Stephen. 1981. Second language acquisition and second language learning. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Krashen, Stephen and Tracy D. Terrell. 1983. The natural approach. Hayward, CA: Alemany Press. Lozanov, Georgi. 1979. Suggestology and outlines of suggestopcdia. New York: Gordon and Breach Science Publishers. Oller, John and Patricia Richard-Amato (eds.). 1983. Methods that work. Rowley, MA: Newbury House. Omaggio, Alice. 1986. Teaching language in context. Boston: Heinle and Heinle. Rassias, John A. 1967. A philosophy of language instruction, or the imporlance of being linguistically earnest. Hanover: Dartmouth University. Rogers, Carl. 1951. Client centered therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. ____ . 1961. On becoming a person: a therapist's view of psychotherapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. Savignon, Sandra, and Margie S. Berns. 1984. Initiatives in communicative language teaching. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. J Stockwell, Robert and J. Donald Bowen. 1965. The sounds of English and Spanish. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Suomi, K. 1976. English voiceless and voiced stops as produced by Finnish and native speakers. Jyvaskyla Contrastive Studies 2. Jyvaskyla: Department of English, Jyvaskyla University. Terrell, Tracy D. 1989. Teaching Spanish pronunciation in a communicative approach. In: Bjarkman and Hammond 1989, pp. 196-214. Widdowson, Henry G. 1978. Teaching language as communication. New York: Oxford University Press.

Smile Life

When life gives you a hundred reasons to cry, show life that you have a thousand reasons to smile

Get in touch

© Copyright 2015 - 2024 PDFFOX.COM - All rights reserved.