Workshop:The Diachrony of Valence: Changes in ... - AEDEAN [PDF]

As a consequence, a drastic increase of labile verbs can be witnessed, i.e. single double-functional verbs whose argumen

0 downloads 3 Views 634KB Size

Recommend Stories


Diachrony of differential argument marking
Open your mouth only if what you are going to say is more beautiful than the silience. BUDDHA

Summary of Changes (PDF - 65KB)
When you talk, you are only repeating what you already know. But if you listen, you may learn something

valence, ville 4 fleurs
If you want to become full, let yourself be empty. Lao Tzu

Greenbergian universals, diachrony, and statistical analyses
Forget safety. Live where you fear to live. Destroy your reputation. Be notorious. Rumi

Valence Bond Theory
You can never cross the ocean unless you have the courage to lose sight of the shore. Andrè Gide

Magnetic localization in mixed-valence manganites
Learning never exhausts the mind. Leonardo da Vinci

Valence, gender, and lateralization of functional brain anatomy in emotion
Don't ruin a good today by thinking about a bad yesterday. Let it go. Anonymous

Electron Configurations, Valence electrons, Types of Bonds
You have to expect things of yourself before you can do them. Michael Jordan

Unconscious Processing of Facial Emotional Valence Relation
Ask yourself: How much of your time during an average week is spent doing things you dislike or that

Idea Transcript


Workshop: The Diachrony of Valence: Changes in Argument Structure 22nd International Conference on Historical Linguistics (ICHL 22), University of Naples, 27-31 July 2015 Organizers:

Werner Abraham (Universities of Vienna & Munich) ([email protected]) Matthias Eitelmann (University of Mainz) ([email protected]) Teresa Fanego (University of Santiago de Compostela) ([email protected]) Elly van Gelderen (Arizona State University) ([email protected]) Dagmar Haumann (University of Agder) ([email protected]) Lars Hellan (University of Trondheim) ([email protected]) Elisabeth Leiss (University of Munich) ([email protected])

Traditionally, argument structure is an integral part of a lexical item's representation encoding the number of arguments the lexical item takes, how the arguments are syntactically realized (e.g. DP, PP, CP) and which semantic roles they instantiate (e.g. Agent, Experiencer, Theme). However, these properties or features are neither synchronically invariable nor diachronically immune to change, especially if a language is affected by largescale typological shifts (Comrie 2006). A showcase example in this respect is English with its development from syntheticity to analyticity. As a consequence, a drastic increase of labile verbs can be witnessed, i.e. single double-functional verbs whose argument structure varies considerably in that they can be used both with an intransitive and a transitive meaning without any overt formal marking, which makes a classification of English verbs as either transitive or intransitive notoriously difficult (van Gelderen 2011). Also, English has enhanced syntactic means for functions formerly expressed morphologically (e.g. causativity: whereas jan-verbs ceased to be productive, periphrastic markers of causativity have taken their place (van Gelderen 2014, García García 2012; Haspelmath 1993), with the most recent addition being constituted by the V NP into V-ing construction). This workshop aims at exploring argument structure from a diachronic (and comparative) perspective focusing on the extent and limitation of variation in the lexical representation and/or the (morpho)syntactic realization of a lexical item's arguments. Two types of processes are of prime interest: those that affect a lexical item's argument structure (transitivization and detransitivization) and those that affect the mapping of a lexical item's arguments onto syntax. 1. Augmentation processes, for example, involve strategies in which the verbal territory of a verb is more or less systematically extended, as is the case with cognate objects that are 1

etymologically and semantically related to the verb they co-occur with, thus occupying the internal argument position of a verb otherwise not taking direct objects (cf. English to smile a smile or Greek ekhárēsan kharàn megálēn sphódra 'they were exceedingly glad with big joy' (cf. Hoeche 2009; Lavidas 2012)), or with 'reaction' objects (cf. English to grunt one's gratitude, to wave an adieu; cf. Felser & Wanner 2001; Levin 1993). 2. Argument reduction processes, for example, affect typically the highest argument of a lexical item, which will either be absent from the lexical item's argument structure, as in anticausatives (cf. German Eis schmilzt schnell 'ice melts fast') and middles (cf. English bananas peel easily), or morphologically 'saturated', as is the case in canonical passives (cf. French il était tué 'he was killed' or Norwegian alt for mye kastes 'too much is thrown away' (cf. Schäfer 2008, Julien 2007, Klingvall 2007, Steinbach 2002, Lødrup 2000)). 3. Argument mapping and remapping becomes evident in syntactic variation, cases in point being complementation patterns (e.g. nominal vs. sentential complements, double object constructions, complements of adjectives), (non)canonical subjects (e.g. quirky subjects, expletive and locative constructions), etc. These phenomena often result from the interplay of changing phonological and morphological conditions, semantic change as well as pragmatic considerations (cf. Abraham 2010, Rudanko 1999, 2011, Hudson 1992, Colleman & De Clerck 2008, Fanego 2004, 2007, Hazout 2008, Barðdal 2006). 4. Additionally, since valence selection may be organized paradigmatically (case morphology contingent also upon aspectual value of (verbal) complement or on the choice between features such as [+/-human/+/-animate]) or syntagmatically (either independent of, or contingent upon, linear position in the clause as in Modern English) a shift from paradigmatic to syntagmatic may be a diachronic choice also (cf. Abraham & Leiss 2015). Against this backdrop, the major questions that this workshop seeks to address are  which factors, diachronically, have impacted the argument structure of lexical items and induced what types of changes  what strategies, if any, are employed to formally mark changes in a lexical item's argument structure and/or the mapping of a lexical item's arguments?  which strategies, if any, compensate for the absence of overt morphosyntactic and morphonological cues/marking?  to what extent are cross-linguistic generalizations possible? 2

On a larger scale, we are interested in the following issues and questions  are there processes that are more (a)typical than others?  are there preferred argument structures? Is there preferred mapping?  argument structure changes in categories other than V and N  why is it that certain types of changes yield (non)uniform results (e.g. argument demotion results in periphrastic passives, morphological passives or middles, whereas argument reduction results in impersonals and anticausatives; reflexivization in both augmentation and reduction processes)  are certain types of adjuncts more prone to reanalysis than others (augmentation of argument structure)?  do labile verbs have labile argument structures?  which types of change affect individual lexical items and which affect classes of lexical items and constructions?  what is the exact interplay of argument structure and event structure (aspect)?  chicken-and-egg questions, e.g. do all changes begin in the lexicon, or can syntax (morphology, semantics, pragmatics) feed argument structure?  descriptions of transitivization, detransitivization, and changes in the mapping Additional questions are:  Since massive syntactic (multiple) fusion of (directional) prepositions (an/ab/vor/zu/auf/ unter/über etc.), on the one hand, and aspectual ge- and simple verbs took place (ver-/ab/vor-/bei-/zer-/über-/unterREDEN) the question rises to which systematic extent this influenced the valence of the new fused verbs. In particular, did the prepositions – themselves a governing category – merge their valence into the resulting fusional complex?  Since prepositional fusion resulted in either (non-focal) prefixal or (focal) particle complexes, and since certain prepositions only yielded focused prefixal morphemes (verSPRECHEN ‘promise’ vs. VORsprechen ‘drop in on’; *anSPRECHEN-ANsprechen), the different results may require different systematic explanations.  What is behind the generality of directional preposition+verb fusion, but not stative preposition+verb?

3

 Is it possible to explain case in terms of features and clear form-function relations in the vein of Jakobson 1957[1971]?  Is there any systematics behind case selection of verba deponentia? Consider Latin utor+ ABLATIVE, which taken by its diathetic form should represent the original AGENT, but which, in fact, represents the direct object of the deponens verb.  Speaking in terms of paradigms, how do languages encode causatives as opposed to decausatives, transitives as opposed to detransitives?  Has there diachronically been a restriction for doubling identical case forms? Consider German jemanden.ACC etwas.ACC lehren; jemanden.ACC etwas.ACC kosten, which are strictly evaded in the spoken dialects. Does this mean that morphological case raises semantic images which are unalignable with one another in a single clause? But consider also Latin aliquem.ACC Latinam linguam.ACC discere, which was valid throughout Classical Latin (but perhaps not in late vulgar Latin?).  What L-contact did and does with valence is an unwritten chapter on this topic. Contributions from pidgins and creoles would be highly welcome. This applies equally to French influence in the period of Middle English.  Given the common insight that under L-contact (i.e. with pidginization and creolization) grammar is transferred to the recipient L a lot less than semantic-lexical information, what role does semantic valence play in the emerging language? Does it influence linearization in a systematic way (linearization aligning with hierarchies of semantic roles)?  Bare datives and the pertinent prepositional constituents (as in English): simply linear alignment or semantic difference?  Is there a particular role laid aside for the reflexive pronominal, either full or as a grammatical suffix, influencing valence emergence synchronically and diachronically? The case of deponens verbs and their semantic case distribution.  Finally, are there instances of morphological valence encoding that are interacting with phonemic syllable status vs. desyllabification due to phonetic changes?

References Abraham, Werner 2010. Types of transitivity. Intransitive objects, and intransitivity – and the logic of their designs: Ways to keep apart derivation in syntax and the lexicon. In: P. Brandt & M. García García (eds.) Transitivity: Form, meaning, acquisition, and 4

processing. [Linguistic Aktuell /Linguistics Today 166]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 15–68. Abraham, Werner & Elisabeth Leiss 2015 (to appear). Syntagmatic vs. paradigmatic case – the methodology for underspecification of case exponency. In: L. Hellan, A. Malchukov & M. Cennamo (eds.) Verbal valency in European languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Barđdal, Jóhanna 2006. Construction-specific properties of syntactic subjects in Icelandic and German. Cognitive Linguistics 17 (1): 39–106. Colleman, Timothy & Bernard De Clerck 2008. Accounting for Ditransitive Constructions with envy and forgive. Functions of Language 15(2): 187–215. Comrie, Bernard 2006. Transitivity pairs, markedness, and diachronic stability. Linguistics 44(2): 303–18. Fanego, Teresa 2004. On reanalysis and actualization in syntactic change: The rise and development of English verbal gerunds. Diachronica 21(1): 5-55. Fanego, Teresa 2007. Drift and the development of sentential complements in British and American English from 1700 to the Present Day. In: J. Pérez-Guerra, D. GonzálezAlvarez, J. Bueno-Alonso & E. Rama-Martínez (eds.). 'Of varying language and opposing creed': New insights into Late Modern English. Bern: Peter Lang, 161–235. Felser, Claudia & Anja Wanner. 2001. The syntax of cognate and other unselected objects. In: N. Dehé & A. Wanner (eds). Structural aspects of semantically complex verbs. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang, 105–30. García García, Luisa 2012. Morphological causatives in Old English: The quest for a vanishing formation. Transactions of the Philological Society 110: 122–48. Gelderen, Elly van 2011. The linguistic cycle: Language change and the language faculty. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Gelderen, Elly van 2014. Changes in psych-verbs: A reanalysis of little v. Ms. Arizona State University, Tempe. Haspelmath, Martin 1993. More on the typology of Inchoative/Causative verb alternations. In: B. Comrie & M. Polinsky (eds.). Causatives and transitivity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 87–120. Hazout, Ilan 2008. On the relation between expletive There and its associate: A reply to Williams. Linguistic Inquiry 39 (1): 117–28. Hoeche, Silke 2009. Cognate object constructions in English. A Cognitive-Linguistic account. Tübingen: Gunter Narr. Hudson, Richard 1992. So-called ‘Double Objects’ and grammatical relations. Language 68 (2): 251–76. Jakobson, Roman 1957[1971]. Shifters, verbal categories and the Russian verb. In: R. Jakobson, Selected writings. Word and language, Vol. 2. Mouton: The Hague, 130-47. Julien, Marit 2007. The relation between morphology and syntax. In: G. Ramchand & Ch. Reiss (eds.). The Oxford handbook of linguistic interfaces. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 209–38. Klingvall, Eva 2007. (De)composing the middle. A Minimalist approach to middles in English and Swedish. PhD dissertation, University of Lund. Lavidas, Nikolaos 2012. Unaccusativity and the diachrony of null and cognate objects in Greek. In E. van Gelderen, J. Barðdal & M. Cennamo (eds.). Argument structure in flux: The Naples-Capri papers. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 307–42. Levin, Beth. 1993. English verb classes and alternations. A preliminary investigation. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. 5

Lødrup, Helge 2000. Exceptions to the Norwegian passive: Unaccusativity, aspect and thematic roles. Norsk Lingvistisk Tidsskrift 18: 37–54. Rudanko, Juhani 1999. Diachronic studies of English complementation patterns. Eighteenth century evidence in tracing the development of verbs and adjectives selecting prepositions and complement clauses. Lanham: University Press of America. Rudanko, Juhani 2011. Changes in complementation in British and American English. Corpusbased studies on non-finite complements in recent English. Houndmills, Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. Schäfer, Florian 2008. The syntax of (anti-)causatives. External arguments in change-of-state contexts. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Steinbach, Markus 2002. Middle voice. A comparative study in the syntax-semantics interface of German. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

6

Smile Life

When life gives you a hundred reasons to cry, show life that you have a thousand reasons to smile

Get in touch

© Copyright 2015 - 2024 PDFFOX.COM - All rights reserved.